Cognitive inertia

Cognitive inertia is the tendency for a particular orientation in how an individual thinks about an issue, belief, or strategy to resist change. In clinical and neuroscientific literature, it is often defined as a lack of motivation to generate distinct cognitive processes needed to attend to a problem or issue. The physics term inertia is to emphasize the rigidity and resistance to change in the method of cognitive processing that has been in use for a significant amount of time. Commonly confused with belief perseverance, cognitive inertia is the perseverance of how one interprets information, not the perseverance of the belief itself.

Cognitive inertia has been causally implicated in disregard of impending threat to one's health or environment, enduring political values, and deficits in task switching. Interest in the phenomenon was largely taken up by economic and industrial psychologists to explain resistance to change in brand loyalty, group brainstorming, and business strategies. In the clinical setting, cognitive inertia has been used as a diagnostic tool for neurodegenerative diseases, depression, and anxiety. Critics have stated that the term oversimplifies resistant thought processes and suggest a more integrative approach that involves motivation, emotion, and developmental factors.

History and methods

Early history

The idea of cognitive inertia has its roots in philosophical epistemology. Early allusions to reduction of cognitive inertia can be found in the Socratic dialogues written by Plato. Socrates builds his argument by using the detractor's beliefs as the premise to his argument's conclusions. In doing so, Socrates reveals the detractor's fallacy of thought inducing the detractor to change their mind or face the reality that their thought processes are in contradiction.[1][2] Ways to combat persistence of cognitive style is also seen in Aristotle's syllogistic method which employs logical consistency of the premises to convince an individual of the conclusion's validity.[3]

In the beginning of the twentieth century, two of the earliest experimental psychologists, Müller and Pilzecker, defined perseveration of thought to be "the tendency of ideas, after once having entered consciousness, to rise freely again in consciousness". Müller described perseveration by illustrating his own inability to inhibit old cognitive strategies with a syllable switching task, while his wife easily switched from one strategy to the next. One of the earliest personality researchers, W. Lankes, more broadly defined perseveration as "being confined to the cognitive side" and possibly "counteracted by strong will".[4] These early ideas of perseveration were the precursor to how the term cognitive inertia would be used to study certain symptoms in patients with neurodegenerative disorders, rumination and depression.[5][6]     

Cognitive psychology

Originally proposed by William J. McGuire in 1960, the theory of cognitive inertia was built upon emergent theories in social psychology and cognitive psychology that centered around cognitive consistency, including Fritz Heider's balance theory and Leon Festinger's cognitive dissonance.[7][8][9] McGuire used the term cognitive inertia to account for an initial resistance to change how an idea was processed after new information, that conflicted with the idea, had been acquired.[7]

In McGuire's initial study involving cognitive inertia, participants gave their opinions of how probable they believed a variety of topics to be. A week later they came back to read messages that related to the topics they had given their opinions on. The messages were presented as factual and were targeted to change the participants' belief in how probable the topics were. Immediately after reading the messages, and one week later, the participants were again assessed on how probable they believed the topics to be. Discomforted by the inconsistency of the related information from the messages and their initial ratings on the topics, McGuire believed the participants would be motivated to shift their probability ratings to be more consistent with the factual messages.[8][10] However, the participants' opinions did not immediately shift toward the information presented in the messages. Instead, a shift towards consistency of thought on the information from the messages and topics grew stronger as time passed, often referred to as "seepage" of information.[11] The lack of change was reasoned to be due to a persistence in the individual's existing thought processes which inhibited their ability to properly re-evaluate their initial opinion, or as McGuire called it, cognitive inertia.[7]

Probabilistic model

Although cognitive inertia was related to many of the consistency theories at the time of its conception, McGuire used a unique method of probability theory and logic to support his hypotheses on change and persistence in cognition.[12][13] Utilizing a syllogistic framework, McGuire proposed that if three issues (a, b and c) were so interrelated that an individual's opinion were in complete support of issues a and b then it would follow their opinion on issue c would be supported as a logical conclusion.[7][3] Furthermore, McGuire proposed if an individual's belief in the probability (p) of the supporting issues (a or b) was changed, then not only would the issue (c) explicitly stated change, but a related implicit issue (d) could be changed as well. More formally:

the required change () on c necessary for maintaining logical consistency among the opinions is

p(c) = p(a & b)

which, assuming that a and b are independent events i.e., that p(a & b) = p(a) p(b) becomes

p(c) = p(a) p(b) + p(a) p(b) + p(a) p(b)

where p(a) and p(b) refer to the initial opinions, before the communication induced changes.

This formula was used by McGuire to show that the effect of a persuasive message on a related, but unmentioned, topic (d) took time to sink in. The assumption was that topic d was predicated on issues a and b, similar to issue c, so if the individual agreed with issue c then so too should they agree with issue d. However, in McGuire's initial study immediate measurement on issue d, after agreement on issues a, b and c, had only shifted half the amount that would be expected to be logically consistent. Follow-up a week later showed that shift in opinion on issue d had shifted enough to be logically consistent with issues a, b, and c, which not only supported the theory of cognitive consistency, but also the initial hurdle of cognitive inertia.[7]

The model was based on probability to account for the idea that individuals do not necessarily assume every issue is 100% likely to happen, but instead there is a likelihood of an issue occurring and the individual's opinion on that likelihood will rest on the likelihood of other interrelated issues.[12]     

Examples

Historical

Group (cognitive) inertia, how a subset of individuals view and process an issue, can have detrimental effects on how emergent and existing issues are handled.[14] In an effort to describe the almost lackadaisical attitude from a large majority of U.S. citizens toward the insurgence of the Spanish flu in 1918, historian Tom Dicke has proposed that cognitive inertia explains why many individuals did not take the flu seriously. At the time most U.S. citizens were familiar with the seasonal flu and viewed it as an irritation that was often easy to treat, infected few and passed quickly with few complications and hardly ever a death. However, this way of thinking about the flu was detrimental to the need for preparation, prevention and treatment of the Spanish flu, due to its quick spread and virulent form, until it was much too late and it became one of the most deadly pandemics in history.[15]

Contemporary

In the more modern period, there is an emerging position that anthropogenic climate change denial is a kind of cognitive inertia. Despite the evidence provided by scientific discovery, there are still those – including nations – who deny its incidence in favor of existing patterns of development.[16]

Geography

To better understand how individuals store and integrate new knowledge with existing knowledge, Friedman and Brown tested participants on where they believed countries and cities to be located latitudinally and then, after giving them the correct information, tested them again on different cities and countries. The majority of participants were able to use the correct information to update their cognitive understanding of geographical locations and place the new locations closer to their correct latitudinal location, which supported the idea that new knowledge not only affects the direct information but also related information. However, there was a small effect of cognitive inertia as some areas were unaffected by the correct information, which the researchers suggested was due to a lack of knowledge linkage in the correct information and new locations presented.[17]

Politics

The persistence of political group membership and ideology is suggested to be due to the inertia of how the individual has perceived the grouping of ideas over time. The individual may accept that something counter to their perspective is true, but it may not be enough to tip the balance of how they process the entirety of the subject.[13]

Governmental organizations can often be resistant or glacially slow to change along with social and technological transformation. Even when evidence of malfunction is clear this form of institutional inertia can persist.[18] Political scientist Francis Fukuyama has asserted that humans imbue intrinsic value on the rules they enact and follow, especially in the larger societal institutions that create order and stability. Despite rapid social change and increasing institutional problems, the value placed on an institution and its rules can mask how well an institution is functioning as well as how that institution could be improved.[19] The inability to change an institutional mindset is supported by the theory of punctuated equilibrium, long periods of deleterious governmental policies punctuated by moments of civil unrest. After decades of economic decline the United Kingdom's referendum to leave to EU was seen as an example of the dramatic movement after a long period of governmental inertia.[18]

Interpersonal roles

The unwavering views of the roles people play in our lives has been suggested as a form of cognitive inertia. When asked how they would feel about a classmate marrying their mother or father undergraduate many students said they would be unable to view their classmate in the role of step-father/mother. Some students went so far as to say that the hypothetical relationship felt like incest.[20]

Role inertia has also been implicated in marriage and likelihood of divorce. Research on couples who co-habitat together before marriage shows they are more likely to get divorced than those who do not. The effect is most seen in a subset of couples who co-habit without first being transparent about future expectations of marriage. Over time cognitive role inertia takes over and the couple marries without fully processing the decision, often with one or both of the partners not fully committed to the idea. The lack of deliberative processing of existing problems and levels of commitment in the relationship can lead to increased stress, arguments, dissatisfaction and divorce.[21]   

In business

Cognitive inertia is regularly referenced in business and management to refer to consumers' continued use of products, a lack of novel ideas in group brainstorming sessions and lack of change in competitive strategies.[22][23][24]

Brand loyalty

Gaining and retaining new customers is an essential part of whether a business succeeds early on. To assess a service, product or likelihood of customer retention many companies will invite their customers to complete satisfaction surveys immediately after the purchase of a product or service. However, unless the satisfaction survey is completed immediately after the point of purchase the customer response is often based on an existing mindset about the company, not the actual quality of experience. Unless the product or service is extremely negative or positive cognitive inertia related to how the customer feels about the company will not be inhibited, even when the product or service is substandard. These satisfaction surveys can lack the information businesses need to improve a service or product that will allow them to survive against the competition.[25]

Brainstorming

Cognitive inertia plays a role in why a lack of ideas are generated during group brainstorming sessions as individuals in a group will often follow an idea trajectory, in which they continue to narrow in on ideas based on the very first idea proposed in the brainstorming session. This idea trajectory actually inhibits the creation of new ideas that was central to the initial formation of the group.[22][26]

In an effort to combat cognitive inertia in group brainstorming, researchers had business students either use a single dialogue or multiple dialogue approach to brainstorming. In the single dialogue version the business students all listed their ideas and created a dialogue around the list, whereas in the multi-dialogue version ideas were placed in subgroups that individuals could choose to enter and talk about and then freely move to another subgroup. The multi-dialogue approach was able to combat cognitive inertia by allowing different ideas to simultaneously be generated in sub-groups and each time an individual switched to a different sub-group they had to change how they were processing the ideas which lead to more novel and high-quality ideas.[26]

Competitive strategies

Adapting cognitive strategies to changing business climates is often integral to whether or not a business succeeds or fails during times of economic stress.[27] In the late 1980s in the UK, real estate agents' cognitive competitive strategies did not shift with signs of an increasingly depressed real estate market, despite their ability to acknowledge the signs of decline.[24] This cognitive inertia at the individual and corporate level has been proposed as reasons to why companies do not adopt new strategies to combat ever increasing decline in the business or take advantage of potential. General Mills' continued operation of mills long after they were no longer necessary is an example of when companies refuse to change the mindset of how the company should operate.[24]

More famously, cognitive inertia in upper management at Polaroid was proposed to be one of the main contributing factors for why competitive strategy for the company was not updated. Management strongly held that consumers wanted high quality physical copies of their photos and that was where the company would make their money. Despite Polaroid's large research and development into the digital market, their inability to refocus their strategy to hardware sales instead of film eventually led to their collapse.[28]

Scenario planning has been one suggestion to combat cognitive inertia when it comes to making strategic decisions to improve business. Individuals come up with different strategies and outline how the scenario could play out taking into account different ways it could go. Scenario planning allows for diverse ideas to be heard as well as the breadth of each scenario, which can help combat relying on existing methods and thinking alternatives are unrealistic.[29]

Management

In a recent review of company archetypes that lead to corporate failure, Habersang, Küberling, Reihlen, and Seckler defined "the laggard" as one who rests on the laurels of the company, believing past success and recognition will shield them from failure. Instead of adapting to changes in the market, "the laggard" assumes that the same strategies that won the company success in the past will do the same in the future. This lag in changing how they think about the company can lead to rigidity in company identity, like Polaroid, conflict in adapting when the sales plummet and resource rigidity. In the case of Kodak, instead of reallocating money to a new product or service strategy, they cut production costs and imitation of competitors both leading to poorer quality product and eventually bankruptcy.[27]

A review of 27 firms integrating the use of big data analytics found cognitive inertia to hamper the widespread implementation, with managers from sectors that did not focus on digital technology seeing the change as unnecessary and cost prohibitive.[30]

Managers with high cognitive flexibility that can change the type of cognitive processing based on the situation at hand are often the most successful in solving novel problems and keeping up with changing circumstances.[31] Interestingly, shift in mental models (disrupting cognitive inertia) during a company crisis are frequently at the lower group level with leaders coming to a consensus with the rest of the workforce in how to process and deal with the crisis, instead of vice versa. It is proposed that leaders can be blinded by their authority and too easily disregard those at the front-line of the problem causing them to reject remunerative ideas.[32]

Applications

Therapy

An inability to change how one thinks about a situation has been implicated as one of the causes of depression. Rumination, or the perseverance of negative thoughts, is often correlated with severity of depression and anxiety. Individuals with high levels of rumination test low on scales of cognitive flexibility and have trouble shifting how they think about a problem or issue even when presented with facts that counter their thinking process.[6]

In a review paper that outlined strategies that are effective for combating depression, the Socratic method was suggested to overcome cognitive inertia. By presenting the patient's incoherent beliefs close together and evaluating with the patient their thought processes behind those beliefs, the therapist is able to help them understand things from a different perspective.[1]

Clinical diagnostics

In nosological literature relating to the symptom or disorder of apathy, clinicians have used cognitive inertia as one of the three main criteria for diagnosis. The description of cognitive inertia differs from its use in cognitive and industrial psychology in that lack of motivation plays a key role. As a clinical diagnostic criterion, Thant and Yager described it as "impaired abilities to elaborate and sustain goals and plans of actions, to shift mental sets, and to use working memory".[33] This definition of apathy is frequently applied to onset of apathy due to neurodegenerative disorders such as Alzheimer's and Parkinson's disease but has also been applied to individuals who have gone through extreme trauma or abuse.[34][35][36]

Neural anatomy and correlates

Cortical

Cognitive inertia has been linked to decreased use of executive function, primarily in the prefrontal cortex, which aids in flexibility of cognitive processes when switching tasks. Delayed response on the implicit associations task (IAT) and Stroop task have been related to an inability to combat cognitive inertia, as participants struggle to switch from one cognitive rule to the next to get the questions right.[37]

Before taking part in an electronic brainstorming session participants were primed with pictures that motivated achievement to combat cognitive inertia. In the achievement primed condition subjects were able to produce more novel high quality ideas and had more use of right frontal cortical areas which were related to decision making and creativity.[38]

Cognitive inertia is commonly used as one of the key dimensions of clinical apathy, described as a lack of motivation to elaborate plans for goal-directed behavior or automated processing.[34] Parkinson's patients whose apathy was measured using the cognitive inertia dimension showed less executive function control than Parkinson's patients without apathy, possibly suggesting more damage to the frontal cortex.[5] Additionally, more damage to the basal ganglia in Parkinson's, Huntington's and other neurodegenerative disorders have been found with patients exhibiting cognitive inertia in relation to apathy when compared to those who do not exhibit apathy. Patients with lesions to the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex have shown reduced motivation to change cognitive strategies and how they view situations, similar to individuals who experience apathy and cognitive inertia after severe or long-term trauma.[35]

Functional connectivity

Nursing home patients who have dementia have been found to have larger reductions in functional brain connectivity primarily in the corpus callosum, important for communication between hemispheres.[34] Cognitive inertia in neurodegenerative patients has also been associated with a decrease in the connection of the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex and posterior parietal area with subcortical areas including the anterior cingulate cortex and basal ganglia.[36] Both findings are suggested to decrease motivation to change one's thought processes or create new goal-directed behavior.[34][36]

Alternative theories

Some researchers have refuted the cognitive perspective of cognitive inertia and suggest a more holistic approach that takes into account the motivations, emotions and attitudes that fortify the existing frame of reference.[39]

Motivated reasoning

The theory of motivated reasoning is proposed to be driven by the individual's motivation to think a certain way, often to avoid thinking negatively about oneself. The individual's own cognitive and emotional biases are commonly used to justify a thought, belief or behavior. Unlike cognitive inertia where an individual's orientation in processing information remains unchanged either due to new information not being fully absorbed or being blocked by a cognitive bias, motivated reasoning may actually change the orientation or keep it the same depending on whether that orientation benefits the individual.[39]

In an extensive online study participant opinions were acquired after two readings about various political issues to assess the role of cognitive inertia. The participants gave their opinion after the first reading and were then assigned a second reading with new information. After being assigned to read more information on the issue that either confirmed or disconfirmed their initial opinion the majority of participants' opinions did not change. When asked about the information in the second reading those who did not change their opinion evaluated the information that supported their initial opinion as stronger than information that disconfirmed their initial opinion. The persistence in how the participants viewed the incoming information was based on their motivation to be correct in their initial opinion, not the persistence of an existing cognitive perspective.[40]

Socio-cognitive inflexibility

From a social psychology perspective individuals are continually forming and shaping beliefs and attitudes about the world around them based on interaction with others. What information the individual attends to is based on prior experience and knowledge of the world. Cognitive inertia is seen not just as a malfunction in updating how information is being processed but that the assumptions about the world and how it works can impede cognitive flexibility.[41] The persistence of the idea of the nuclear family has been proposed as a socio-cognitive inertia. Despite the changing trends in family structure including multi-generational, single-parent, blended and same-sex parent families, the normative idea of a family has centered around the mid-twentieth century idea of a nuclear family (i.e. mother, father, and children). Various social influences are proposed to maintain the inertia of this viewpoint, including media portrayals, persistence of working-class gender roles, unchanged domestic roles despite working mothers and familial pressure to conform.[42]

The phenomenon of cognitive inertia in brainstorming groups has been argued to be due to other psychological effects such as fear of disagreeing with an authority figure in the group, fear of new ideas being rejected and the majority of speech being attributed to the minority of group members.[43] Internet-based brainstorming groups have been found to produce more ideas of high-quality because it overcomes the problem of speaking up and fear of idea rejection.[26]       

See also

References

  1. Overholser, James C. (2011). "Collaborative Empiricism, Guided Discovery, and the Socratic Method: Core Processes for Effective Cognitive Therapy". Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice. 18 (1): 62–66. doi:10.1111/j.1468-2850.2011.01235.x. ISSN 0969-5893.
  2. Jones, Jean G.; Simmons, Herbert W. (2017). Persuasion in society. New York, NY: Routledge. ISBN 9781138825659. OCLC 975176277.
  3. Rapp, Christof (August 2012). "Chapter 22: Aristotle on the Moral Psychology of Persuasion". In Sheilds, Christopher (ed.). The Oxford Handbook of Aristotle. Oxford University Press. pp. 589–610. ISBN 9780195187489.
  4. Bernstein, E. (1924). "Quickness and intelligence: An enquiry concerning the existence of a general speed factor". British Journal of Psychology, Monograph Supplement. 3 (7): 1–55. OCLC 3945730.
  5. Santangelo, Gabriella; D'Iorio, Alfonsina; Maggi, Gianpaolo; Cuoco, Sofia; Pellecchia, Maria Teresa; Amboni, Marianna; Barone, Paolo; Vitale, Carmine (2018). "Cognitive correlates of "pure apathy" in Parkinson's disease". Parkinsonism & Related Disorders. 53: 101–104. doi:10.1016/j.parkreldis.2018.04.023. ISSN 1353-8020. PMID 29706433.
  6. Koval, Peter; Kuppens, Peter; Allen, Nicholas B.; Sheeber, Lisa (2012). "Getting stuck in depression: The roles of rumination and emotional inertia". Cognition & Emotion. 26 (8): 1412–1427. doi:10.1080/02699931.2012.667392. ISSN 0269-9931. PMID 22671768. S2CID 14380665.
  7. McGuire, William J. (1960). "Cognitive consistency and attitude change". The Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology. 60 (3): 345–353. doi:10.1037/h0048563.
  8. Abelson, Robert P., ed. (1968). Theories of cognitive consistency; a sourcebook. Rand McNally. OCLC 643510119.
  9. McGuire, William J. (2013). "An Additional Future for Psychological Science". Perspectives on Psychological Science. 8 (4): 414–423. doi:10.1177/1745691613491270. ISSN 1745-6916. PMID 26173120.
  10. McGuire, William J. (1966). "The Current Status of Cognitive Consistency Theories". In Feldman, Shel (ed.). Cognitive Consistency: Motivational Antecedents and Behavioral Consequents. Academic Press Inc. pp. 1–46. ISBN 0122526503.
  11. Cook, Thomas D.; Burd, John R.; Talbert, Terence L. (1970). "Cognitive, Behavioral and Temporal Effects of Confronting a Belief with Its Costly Action Implications". Sociometry. 33 (3): 358–369. doi:10.2307/2786163. ISSN 0038-0431. JSTOR 2786163.
  12. Cameron, Kenzie A. (2008). "A practitioner's guide to persuasion: An overview of 15 selected persuasion theories, models and frameworks". Patient Education and Counseling. 74 (3): 309–317. doi:10.1016/j.pec.2008.12.003. ISSN 0738-3991. PMID 19136229.
  13. Jost, John T.; Hardin, Curtis D. (2010-11-12). "On the Structure and Dynamics of Human Thought: The Legacy of William J. McGuire for Social and Political Psychology". Political Psychology. 32 (1): 21–58. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9221.2010.00794.x. ISSN 0162-895X. S2CID 18780228.
  14. Zárate, Michael A.; Reyna, Christine; Alvarez, Miriam J. (2019), "Cultural inertia, identity, and intergroup dynamics in a changing context", Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, Elsevier, pp. 175–233, doi:10.1016/bs.aesp.2018.11.001, ISBN 9780128171677, S2CID 149548667
  15. Dicke, T. (2014-06-22). "Waiting for the Flu: Cognitive Inertia and the Spanish Influenza Pandemic of 1918-19". Journal of the History of Medicine and Allied Sciences. 70 (2): 195–217. doi:10.1093/jhmas/jru019. ISSN 0022-5045. PMC 7313928. PMID 24957069.
  16. Bellamy, Brent Ryan (2017). "Chapter 8: The Inertia of Energy". In Huebener, Paul; O'Brien, Susie; Porter, Anthony R. D.; Stockdale, Liam P. D.; Zhou, Yanqiu Rachel (eds.). Time, Globalization and Human Experience: Interdisciplinary Explorations. New York, NY: Routledge. pp. 145–159. ISBN 9781138697331.
  17. Friedman, Alinda; Brown, Norman R. (2000). "Updating geographical knowledge: Principles of coherence and inertia". Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition. 26 (4): 900–914. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.26.4.900. ISSN 0278-7393. PMID 10946370.
  18. Zantvoort, Bart (2016-10-02). "Political inertia and social acceleration". Philosophy & Social Criticism. 43 (7): 707–723. doi:10.1177/0191453716669195. ISSN 0191-4537. S2CID 152040911.
  19. Fukuyama, Francis, Political order and political decay : from the industrial revolution to the globalization of democracy, ISBN 978-1-4915-8487-3, OCLC 904401704
  20. Catton, William R. (1969). "What's in a Name? A Study of Role Inertia". Journal of Marriage and Family. 31 (1): 15–18. doi:10.2307/350001. ISSN 0022-2445. JSTOR 350001.
  21. Stanley, Scott M.; Rhoades, Galena Kline; Markman, Howard J. (2006). "Sliding Versus Deciding: Inertia and the Premarital Cohabitation Effect*". Family Relations. 55 (4): 499–509. doi:10.1111/j.1741-3729.2006.00418.x. ISSN 1741-3729. S2CID 42763576.
  22. Jablin, Fredric M.; Seibold, David R. (1978). "Implications for problem‐solving groups of empirical research on 'brainstorming': A critical review of the literature". Southern Speech Communication Journal. 43 (4): 327–356. doi:10.1080/10417947809372391. ISSN 0361-8269.
  23. Han, Heesup; Kim, Yunhi; Kim, Eui-Keun (2011). "Cognitive, affective, conative, and action loyalty: Testing the impact of inertia". International Journal of Hospitality Management. 30 (4): 1008–1019. doi:10.1016/j.ijhm.2011.03.006. ISSN 0278-4319.
  24. Narayanan, V.K.; Zane, Lee J.; Kemmerer, Benedict (2010-10-13). "The Cognitive Perspective in Strategy: An Integrative Review". Journal of Management. 37 (1): 305–351. doi:10.1177/0149206310383986. ISSN 0149-2063. S2CID 145139445.
  25. Mattila, Anna S. (2003-06-01). "The Impact of Cognitive Inertia on Postconsumption Evaluation Processes". Journal of the Academy of Marketing Science. 31 (3): 287–299. doi:10.1177/0092070303031003006. ISSN 0000-0000. S2CID 144115253.
  26. Dennis, Alan R.; Valacich, Joseph S.; Carte, Traci A.; Garfield, Monica J.; Haley, Barbara J.; Aronson, Jay E. (1997). "Research Report: The Effectiveness of Multiple Dialogues in Electronic Brainstorming". Information Systems Research. 8 (2): 203–211. doi:10.1287/isre.8.2.203. ISSN 1047-7047.
  27. Habersang, Stefanie; Küberling, Jill; Reihlen, Markus; Seckler, Christoph (2019). "A Process Perspective on Organizational Failure: A Qualitative Meta-Analysis". Journal of Management Studies. 56 (1): 19–56. doi:10.1111/joms.12341.
  28. Tripsas, Mary; Gavetti, Giovanni (2000). "Capabilities, Cognition, and Inertia: Evidence From Digital Imaging". Strategic Management Journal. 21 (10/11): 1147–1161. doi:10.1002/1097-0266(200010/11)21:10/11<1147::AID-SMJ128>3.0.CO;2-R.
  29. Wright, George; Goodwin, Paul (1999). "Future-focused thinking: combining scenario planning with decision analysis". Journal of Multi-Criteria Decision Analysis. 8 (6): 311–321. doi:10.1002/1099-1360(199911)8:6<311::aid-mcda256>3.0.co;2-t. ISSN 1057-9214.
  30. Mikalef, Patrick; van de Wetering, Rogier; Krogstie, John (2018), "Big Data Enabled Organizational Transformation: The Effect of Inertia in Adoption and Diffusion", Business Information Systems, Springer International Publishing, pp. 135–147, doi:10.1007/978-3-319-93931-5_10, hdl:11250/2589310, ISBN 9783319939308, S2CID 51611480
  31. Laureiro‐Martínez, Daniella; Brusoni, Stefano (2018-03-15). "Cognitive flexibility and adaptive decision‐making: Evidence from a laboratory study of expert decision makers". Strategic Management Journal. 39 (4): 1031–1058. doi:10.1002/smj.2774. ISSN 0143-2095.
  32. Carrington, David J.; Combe, Ian A.; Mumford, Michael D. (2019). "Cognitive shifts within leader and follower teams: Where consensus develops in mental models during an organizational crisis". The Leadership Quarterly. 30 (3): 335–350. doi:10.1016/j.leaqua.2018.12.002. ISSN 1048-9843.
  33. Thant, Thida; Yager, Joel (2019). "Updating Apathy: Using Research Domain Criteria to Inform Clinical Assessment and Diagnosis of Disorders of Motivation". The Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease. 207 (9): 707–714. doi:10.1097/NMD.0000000000000860. ISSN 0022-3018. PMID 30256334. S2CID 52822581.
  34. Agüera-Ortiz, Luis; Hernandez-Tamames, Juan A.; Martinez-Martin, Pablo; Cruz-Orduña, Isabel; Pajares, Gonzalo; López-Alvarez, Jorge; Osorio, Ricardo S.; Sanz, Marta; Olazarán, Javier (2016-07-18). "Structural correlates of apathy in Alzheimer's disease: a multimodal MRI study". International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry. 32 (8): 922–930. doi:10.1002/gps.4548. ISSN 0885-6230. PMID 27428560. S2CID 34539433.
  35. Leach, John (2018). "'Give-up-itis' revisited: Neuropathology of extremis". Medical Hypotheses. 120: 14–21. doi:10.1016/j.mehy.2018.08.009. ISSN 0306-9877. PMID 30220334. S2CID 52282470.
  36. Pagonabarraga, Javier; Kulisevsky, Jaime; Strafella, Antonio P; Krack, Paul (2015). "Apathy in Parkinson's disease: clinical features, neural substrates, diagnosis, and treatment". The Lancet Neurology. 14 (5): 518–531. doi:10.1016/s1474-4422(15)00019-8. hdl:10609/92806. ISSN 1474-4422. PMID 25895932. S2CID 43614772.
  37. Messner, Claude; Vosgerau, Joachim (2010). "Cognitive Inertia and the Implicit Association Test". Journal of Marketing Research. 47 (2): 374–386. doi:10.1509/jmkr.47.2.374. ISSN 0022-2437. S2CID 144453510.
  38. Minas, Randall K.; Dennis, Alan R.; Potter, Robert F.; Kamhawi, Rasha (2017-11-06). "Triggering Insight: Using Neuroscience to Understand How Priming Changes Individual Cognition during Electronic Brainstorming". Decision Sciences. 49 (5): 788–826. doi:10.1111/deci.12295. ISSN 0011-7315.
  39. Kunda, Ziva (1990). "The case for motivated reasoning". Psychological Bulletin. 108 (3): 480–498. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.108.3.480. ISSN 1939-1455. PMID 2270237. S2CID 9703661.
  40. Stanley, Matthew L.; Henne, Paul; Yang, Brenda W.; De Brigard, Felipe (2019-01-16). "Resistance to Position Change, Motivated Reasoning, and Polarization". Political Behavior. 42 (3): 891–913. doi:10.1007/s11109-019-09526-z. ISSN 1573-6687. S2CID 150286954.
  41. Stein, Johan (1997-09-01). "How Institutions Learn: A Socio-Cognitive Perspective". Journal of Economic Issues. 31 (3): 729–740. doi:10.1080/00213624.1997.11505962. ISSN 0021-3624.
  42. Uhlmann, Allon J. (2005). "The Dynamics of Stasis: Historical Inertia in The Evolution of the Australian Family". The Australian Journal of Anthropology. 16 (1): 31–46. doi:10.1111/j.1835-9310.2005.tb00108.x. ISSN 1035-8811.
  43. Dillehay, Ronald C.; Insko, Chester A.; Smith, M. Brewster (1966). "Logical consistency and attitude change". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 3 (6): 646–654. doi:10.1037/h0023286. ISSN 1939-1315. PMID 5939001.
This article is issued from Wikipedia. The text is licensed under Creative Commons - Attribution - Sharealike. Additional terms may apply for the media files.